Fathers as attachment figures
Studies have found that the father is a child's preferred attachment figure in approximately 5–20% of cases.[1][2][3] Fathers and mothers may react differently to the same behaviour in an infant, and the infant may react to the parents' behaviour differently depending on which parent performs it.[4]
Theoretical perspectives
Psychoanalytic theory
Sigmund Freud postulated that early in life, a young infant's primary attachment object would be its mother because the mother fulfills the infant's oral desires through feeding.[5] However, he believed that the father begins to play an important role in development when the child enters the phallic stage of development, which generally occurs approximately at the age of three.[5] According to Sigmund Freud, during the phallic stage, children begin to form an incestuous desire for their opposite-sex parent and an antagonistic rivalry with their same-sex parent.[5] The resolution of this stage of development occurs when the child, usually at the age of six, renounces their desire for their opposite-sex parent and begins to identify with their same-sex parent.[5]
Predictors and correlations of father-child attachment
Gender
Male children are more likely to prefer their father as an attachment figure than female children are.[4]
Age
Children are more likely to be attached to their father during their late childhood to early adolescence.[4] Infants and young adults are less likely to seek attachment to their fathers.[4]
Temperament
The infant's temperament can influence the role that the father plays in child rearing. A great deal of research has shown that fathers are less likely to be involved with their infant if the infant has a difficult temperament.[6][7][8] Furthermore, one study suggests that this lack of paternal involvement in the case of fussy infants may harm the mother's relationship with her child if the mother believes that the paternal care giving role is important.[9]
Paternal sensitivity
Research on whether or not paternal sensitivity has an effect on the security of father-child attachment has produced mixed results. Some studies have shown significant correlations between paternal sensitivity and secure father-child attachments.[10][11] One study suggests that a father's sensitivity while playing with his child is as important to the child-father attachment relationship as maternal sensitivity during caregiving is for the child-mother attachment relationship.[12] Furthermore, one study has found that the harm of a father's frightening behaviours on a child's future development can be mitigated if the father also has high levels of paternal sensitivity.[13] Having a father who is both insensitive and exhibits frightening behaviours during infancy has been linked to emotional under-regulation at the age of two as well as attention problems at the age of seven.[13] However, other studies have failed to find a correlation between paternal sensitivity and infant-father attachment.[14][15]
The father's own attachment style
The level of attachment security that a father had with his parents when he was a child may influence his own child's attachment security in the sense that his child may end up having a similar level of attachment security. However, this effect is most likely to occur when the father has sole custody of his child.[16] The father's level of attachment security in his adult relationships may also have an indirect effect on the child-father attachment. This is because fathers who have a secure attachment style in adult relationships tend to have lower levels of parenting stress, lower levels of abuse potential, and a greater amount of knowledge about child development.[17] Lower levels of parenting stress particularly have been found to be correlated with higher levels of attachment security in the father-child attachment relationship.[17]
The father's beliefs about the importance of paternal caregiving
One study has found that whether or not the father's beliefs about the importance of paternal caregiving affect the security of the father-child attachment relationship depends on the temperament of the child.[9] Fathers who believe that the paternal caregiving role is important are only more likely to have securely attached infants in the case of very fussy infants.[9] The authors of this study suggest that the reason that this finding is limited to fussy infants could be because difficult children may be more susceptible to both the positive and the negative effects of their rearing environments than non-difficult children.[9][18]
Spousal factors
Two important spousal factors have been found to have a relationship with father-infant attachment security: marital intimacy and supportive co-parenting. In the case of marital intimacy, research has shown that a higher level of marital intimacy is correlated with a more secure father-infant attachment relationship, and that deteriorating marital intimacy is correlated with negative father-child interaction.[19] In terms of supportive co-parenting, one study has found that both observed and self-reported supportive co-parenting predict a more secure father-infant attachment, even after controlling for father sensitivity.[20] This study did not find a correlation between supportive co-parenting and the mother-infant attachment relationship.[20] However, when the infants in this study were divided by gender and analyzed separately, the results were quite different.[20] When the infant was a boy, observed supportive co-parenting was associated with greater mother-infant attachment security and self-reported supportive co-parenting was related to greater father-infant attachment security.[20] When the infant was a girl, supportive co-parenting was related to neither the infant-mother attachment security nor the infant-father attachment security.[20] The authors of this study suggest that one possible explanation for this gender difference is that parents might try to hide their marital conflict more from their daughters than from their sons[21] causing sons to be more sensitive to differences in whether or not their parents engage in supportive co-parenting.[20]
Unique properties
Generally, research has found that, compared to mothers, fathers tend to fill more of a "play-mate" role for their children, rather than a "caregiver" role. Various studies have found that fathers are more likely than mothers to encourage risk-taking and exploration in their children by engaging their young children in physical play and initiating games that are both unpredictable and enjoyable.[22][23][24][25][26][27] Most infants tend to prefer contact with their mothers when they are distressed and seeking comfort, and contact with their fathers when they are in more positive emotional states and seeking play.[28] Furthermore, a study on frightening behaviours has suggested that young children may internalize interactions with their mother and father differently in a way that reflects these different roles.[13] The study found that when sensitive fathers engage in frightening behaviours, the paternal sensitivity mitigates the negative effects of frightening behaviours, however, when mothers engage in frightening behaviours, maternal sensitivity does not mitigate the negative effects.[13] Some researchers believe that this occurs because frightening behaviours are more compatible with the father's play-mate role than they are with the mother's role of providing comfort when the child is distressed.[13] They suggest that it may be more harmful to a child when the person who the child is supposed to go to when distressed is the one who is causing the distress.[13] In fact, these researchers believe that children may respond more positively to frightening behaviours exhibited by the father when their mother is nearby, although this has yet to be proven.[13] Research has shown that fathers, compared to mothers, are less able to detect low levels of infant distress,[29] which may contribute to the mother's greater tendency to fulfill more of a "caregiving" role for the child. However, when fathers are required to act as caregivers for their child, such as in situations when the mother is working, they soon become very capable in fulfilling all of the responsibilities that traditionally belong to the mother, and they even begin to serve as a secure base for their child when the child explores its environment.[28]
Effects of father-child attachment on future development
Secure vs. insecure attachment to father
Children who have a secure attachment to their father tend to have improved developmental outcomes in a variety of ways including having improved social abilities with their peers, having fewer problem behaviours, and the paternal effects on developing a greater level of emotional self-regulation are especially significant.[30][31][32][33][34] Furthermore, one study found that 11–13 month old infants who were securely attached to their fathers were more sociable with strangers in the Strange Situation Test.[35] The mother-infant attachment security, on the other hand, had no effect on sociability with strangers in this study.[35] In addition, having a secure father-child attachment relationship can help compensate for potentially harmful effects resulting from an insecure mother-child attachment relationship.[36][37]
Absent father/no attachment
One study has found that paternal absence has various negative effects on children.[38] These negative effects include lower levels of school achievement, heightened risk-taking behaviours, and higher levels of aggression in boys.[38] Another study found that male infants who have little to no interaction with their fathers have significantly lower levels of social responsiveness, fewer secondary circular reactions, and lower levels of preferences for novel stimuli, while female infants were unaffected by the absence of their father.[39] This second study, however, only looks at mother-only single-parent families in low socioeconomic circumstances so the results may not be generalizable to the greater population.[39]
References
- Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of attachment.Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
- Freeman, H., & Brown, B.B. (2001). Primary attachment to parents and peers during adolescence: Differences by attachment style. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30(6), 655–674.
- Trinke, S.J., & Bartholomew, K. (1997). Hierarchies of attachment relationships in young adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14(5), 603–625.
- Freeman, H., Newland, L.A., & Coyle, D.D. (2010). New directions in father attachment. Early Child Development and Care, 180(1-2), 1-8
- Shaffer, D. (2008). Social and personality development. (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: WADSWORTH CENGAGE Learning.
- Manlove, E. E., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2002). Caring for infant daughters and sons in dual-earner households: Maternal reports of father involvement in weekday time and tasks. Infant and Child Development, 11, 305–320.
- McBride, B. A., Schoppe, S. J., & Rane, T. R. (2002). Child characteristics, parenting stress, and parental involvement: Father versus mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 998–1011.
- Volling, B., & Belsky, J. (1991). Multiple determinants of father involvement during infancy in dual-earner and single-earner families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 461–474.
- Wong, M. S., Mangelsdorf, S. C., Brown, G. L., Neff, C., & Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J. (2009). Parental beliefs, infant temperament, and marital quality: Associations with infant-mother and infant-father attachment. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 828 – 838. doi:10.1037/a0016491
- Cox, M. J., Owen, M. T., Henderson, V. K., & Margand, N. A. (1992). Prediction of infant-father and infant-mother attachment. Developmental Psychology, 28, 474-483
- Goossens, F. A., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1990). Quality of infants' attachments to professional caregivers: Relation to infant-parent attachment and day-care characteristics. Child Development,61, 832-837.
- Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child–father attachment relationship: Father's sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development, 11, 301–337. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00202
- Hazen, N. L., McFarland, L., Jacobvitz, D., & Boyd-Soisson, E. (2010). Fathers' frightening behaviours and sensitivity with infants: Relations with fathers' attachment representations, father-infant attachment, and children's later outcomes. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 51– 69. doi:10.1080/03004430903414703
- Easterbrooks, M. A., & Goldberg, W . A. (1984).Toddler development in the family: Impact of father involvement and parenting characteristics. Child Development, 55, 740-752.
- Grossmann, K., & Grossmann, K. E. (1992). Newborn behavior, the quality of early parenting and later toddler-parent relationships in a group of German infants. In J. K. Nugent, B. M. Lester, &.T. B. Brazelton (Eds.), The cultural context of infancy (Vol. 2, pp. 3-38). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
- Bernier, A., & Miljkovitch, R. (2009). Intergenerational transmission of attachment in father– child dyads: The case of single parenthood. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 170, 31–51.
- Howard, K.S. (2010). Paternal attachment, parenting beliefs and children's attachment. Early Child Development and Care, 180(1-2), p. 157-171
- Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300–304
- Belsky J, Youngblade L, Rovine M & Volling B (1991) Patterns of marital change and parent-child interaction. Journal of Marriage and Family 53, 487–498.
- Brown, G. L., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., Mangelsdorf, S. C., & Neff, C. (2010). Observed and reported supportive coparenting as predictors of infant-mother and infant-father attachment security. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 121–137. doi:10.1080/03004430903415015
- Cummings, E.M., Davies, P.T., & Simpson, K.S. (1994). Marital conflict, gender, and children's appraisals and coping efficacy as mediators of child adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 141–149.
- Parke, R.D., & Burielm R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg, (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed. pp. 429-504). New York: Wiley.
- Bretherton, I., Lambert, J.D., & Golby, B. (2005). Involved fathers of preschool children as seen by themselves and their wives: Accounts of attachment, socialization, and companionship. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 229–251.
- Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father–child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193–219.
- Cabrera, N., Fitzgerald, H.E., Bradley, R.H., & Roggman, L. (2007). Modeling the dynamics of paternal influence on children over the life course. Applied Developmental Science, 11(4), 185–189.
- Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K.E., Kindler, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2008). A wider view of attachment and exploration: The influence of mothers and fathers on the development of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood. In J. Cassidy and P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 880–905). New York: Guilford Press
- Newland, L.A., Coyl, D.D., & Freeman, H.S. (2008). Predicting preschoolers' attachment security from fathers' involvement, internal working models, and use of social support. Early Child Development and Care, 178(7&8), 785–801.
- Lamb, M.E. (1997). The role of the father in child development (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley
- Hrdy, S.B. (1999). Mother nature: Maternal instincts and how they shape the human species. New York: Ballantine.
- Cabrera, N.J., Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Bradley, R.H., Hofferth, S., & Lamb, M.E. (2000). Fatherhood is the twenty-first century. Child Development, 71. 127-136.
- Coley, R.L., & Medeiros, B.L. (2007) Reciprocal longitudinal relations between non-resident father involvement and adolescent delinquency. Child Development, 78, 132-147
- Lieberman, M., Doyle, A.B., & Markiewicz, D. (1999). Developmental patterns of security of attachment to mother and father in late child-hood and early adolescence: Association with peer relations. Child Development, 70, 202-213
- Pleck, J.H., & Masciadrelli, B.P. (2004). Paternal involvement by U.S. resident fathers: Levels, sources, and consequences. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development, 4th ed. (pp. 222-271). New York: Wiley
- Diener, M., Mangelsdorf, S., McHale, J., & Frosch, C. (1998) Mother-father differences and paternal correlates of infant emotion regulation. Infant Behaviour and Development, 21, pp. 46
- Lamb ME, Hwang CP, Frodi A. Frodi M. (1982). Security of mother- and father-infant attachment and its relation to sociability with strangers in traditional and non-traditional Swedish families. Infant Behavior and Development 5:3.55-367.
- Main, M., & Weston, D.R. (1981). The quality of the toddler's relationship to mother and to father: Related to conflict and the readiness to establish new relationships. Child Development, 52, 932-940
- Verschueren, K., & Marcoen, A. (1999). Representations of self and socioemotional competence in kindergarten: Combined and differential effects of attachment to mother and to father. Child Development, 70, 183-201
- McLanahan, S., & Teitler, J. (1999). The consequences of father absence. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), Parenting and child development in "nontraditional" families (pp. 83−102). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate Publishers.
- Pedersen, F.A., Rubenstein, J.L., & Yarrow, L.J. (1979). Infant development in father-absent families, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 135:1 p.51-61